MANAGERIAL DETERMINANTS OF BANK RISK TAKING. THE INFLUENCE OF CEO AFFECTIVE TRAITS JUAN MANUEL DE LA FUENTE SABATÉ Catedrático de Universidad Universidad de Burgos Facultad de CC.EE. y Empresariales C/ Los Parralillos, s/n 09001 Burgos SPAIN Tel: +34 947 25 89 73 Fax: +34 947 25 89 60 e-mail: jmfuente@ubu.es JUAN BAUTISTA DELGADO GARCÍA Profesor Ayudante Doctor Universidad de Burgos Facultad de CC.EE. y Empresariales C/ Los Parralillos, s/n 09001 Burgos SPAIN Tel: +34 947 25 90 32 Fax: +34 947 25 89 60 e-mail: jbdelgado@ubu.es ESTHER DE QUEVEDO PUENTE Profesor Titular de Escuela Universitaria Universidad de Burgos Facultad de CC.EE. y Empresariales C/ Los Parralillos, s/n 09001 Burgos SPAIN Tel: +34 947 25 90 33 Fax: +34 947 25 89 60 e-mail: equev@ubu.es Área temática: C) Direccción y Organización Palabras clave: discrecionalidad directiva; afectos; upper echelons; risk taking bancario. 1 RASGOS DIRECTIVOS DETERMINANTES DEL RISK TAKING BANCARIO. LA INFLUENCIA DE LOS RASGOS AFECTIVOS DEL CEO RESUMEN La investigación en banca ha desarrollado numerosas explicaciones de los factores determinantes del risk taking bancario. Sin embargo, esta línea de investigación ha ignorado el papel de las características demográficas y de los rasgos psicológicos de los directivos en sus decisiones estratégicas. Este artículo analiza la influencia de los rasgos afectivos de los CEOs en el risk taking bancario. Las hipótesis se han testado en una muestra de bancos y cajas de ahorros españoles. Nuestros resultados muestran que los rasgos afectivos negativos de los CEOs están relacionados con un menor nivel de riesgo, mientras que los afectos positivos no parecen influir en el nivel de riesgo. 2 MANAGERIAL DETERMINANTS OF BANK RISK TAKING. THE INFLUENCE OF CEO AFFECTIVE TRAITS ABSTRACT Research in banking has generated numerous explanations of determinants of bank risk taking. However, this line of research has traditionally ignored the role of managers’ background characteristics and psychological traits in shaping their strategic choices. This article analyzes the influence of the emotional traits of CEOs on bank risk taking. The hypotheses are tested on a sample of Spanish banks and savings banks. Our results show that managers’ negative affective traits are related to lower risk taking. Positive affective traits do not seem to influence on the level of risk. 3 INTRODUCCIÓN El análisis de los determinantes del riesgo ha sido una de las preocupaciones más frecuentes en la literatura bancaria. Dejando a un lado las condiciones relacionadas con la estructura de los mercados 1 o con las diferencias regulatorias 2 entre países, la investigación empírica se ha centrado en múltiples determinantes organizativos e individuales. A partir del trabajo de Merton (1977), un gran número de trabajos se ha basado en la influencia de la existencia de un sistema de fondo de garantía de depósitos de prima fija sobre los incentivos de los accionistas de los bancos para incrementar el nivel de riesgo bancario, lo que algunos autores han llamado el problema del riesgo moral. En este sentido, el fondo de garantía de depósitos se puede considerar como un subsidio para los accionistas con forma de opción put, cuyo valor aumenta con el riesgo asumido por el banco, lo que a su vez incrementa el incentivo de los accionistas para incrementar el nivel de riesgo bancario (Merton, 1977; 1978; Dothau y Williams, 1980). Otros autores han considerado el efecto de la forma organizativa y de la estructura de capital en el risk taking bancario. Esty (1997a and b) encontró que las entidades financieras constituidas como sociedades anónimas (stock thrifts) mantenían un mayor nivel de riesgo que las entidades financieras constituidas como mutuas (mutual thrifts), y que la transformación de estas mutuas en sociedades anónimas conllevaba un aumento en sus niveles de riesgo. Barth, Hudson y Jahera (1995) y García Marco y Robles Fernández (2002) han obtenido unos resultados similares en muestras de cajas de ahorros tejanas y de bancos y cajas de ahorros españoles. Schrand y Unal (1998) también encontraron que las entidades financieras con forma de mutuas que se convertían en sociedades anónimas aumentaban el nivel riesgo después de esta transformación. La estructura de propiedad y la naturaleza del principal accionista también se han mostrado determinantes de los niveles de risk taking. El trabajo de Laeven (2002) mostraba que el nivel de riesgo era superior en bancos de propiedad concentrada. Además cuando el principal accionista era una compañía u otra institución financiera el nivel de riesgo era superior que cuando la entidad era de propiedad estatal o familiar. Estas investigaciones, entre otras, muestran interesantes explicaciones de los determinantes del risk taking bancario, pero han centrado básicamente sus argumentos en el interés de los accionistas. Sin embargo, es la dirección la encargada de adoptar las decisiones que condicionan el nivel de riesgo de las entidades financieras. Siguiendo este argumento, un segundo grupo de trabajos (Saunders, Strock y Travlos, 1990; Gorton y Rosen, 1995; Brewer y Saidenberg, 1996; Chen, Steiner y White, 1998; Cebenoyan, Cooperman y Register, 1999; 1 Véanse Rhoades y Rutz (1982); Keeley (1990); Shy y Stenbacka (2004) y Brewer y Jackson (2006), entre otros. Véanse Dothau y Williams, 1980; Suárez (1998); Matutes y Vives (2000); Hendrickson y Nichols (2001); Barth, Caprio y Levine (2004) y González (2005), entre otros. 2 4 Anderson y Fraser, 2000) han analizado el papel de la propiedad de los directivos. Los resultados de estas investigaciones posteriores han mostrado relaciones diversas,3 pero en todas ellas se observa una influencia significativa de la propiedad de la dirección en los niveles de risk taking. Estos resultados reflejan la influencia de las decisiones de la dirección en los niveles de riesgo bancario. Sin embargo, esta literatura no contempla la existencia de diferencias idiosincrásicas entre los directivos que también puede influir en el risk taking bancario. En las últimas décadas son numerosos los investigadores en dirección estratégica que se han preocupado por comprender en modo en que las características de los ejecutivos pueden condicionar sus decisiones estratégicas, lo que se reflejaría en el comportamiento de la organización. Esta línea de investigación, frecuentemente denominada perspectiva “Upper Echelons”, se ha centrado en el papel de ciertas características demográficas de los CEOs (por ejemplo, edad, experiencia, experiencia funcional y nivel y tipo de educación; Thomas et al., 1991; Hitt y Tyler, 1991; Jensen y Zajac, 2004) y de sus rasgos psicológicos (por ejemplo, aversión al riesgo, alcance de control, flexibilidad y necesidad de logro; Gupta y Govindarajan, 1984; Miller et al., 1986) en la explicación de determinados comportamientos empresariales como la asunción de riesgos. Nuestro estudio sigue esta línea de investigación e incorpora un nuevo rasgo psicológico en la investigación “Upper Echelons”: el papel de los rasgos afectivos del CEO. La literatura en emociones emplea diferentes términos como afecto, estado de ánimo o emoción que son en ocasiones difíciles de distinguir. En nuestra investigación seguimos a Forgas (1991) y otros autores que utilizan “afecto” como un término general que engloba tanto emociones como estados de ánimo. Los “rasgos afectivos” hacen referencia a diferencias estables en la tendencia a experimentar afecto positivos—e.g., excitación, entusiasmo—o negativos—e.g., culpa, irritación— (Rusting, 1998; Watson y Clark, 1984; Watson, Clark y Tellegen, 1988). El trabajo presenta la siguiente estructura. El apartado siguiente proporciona una revisión de la investigación que ha mostrado la influencia de las características del directivo en la propensión a asumir riesgos. A continuación se introducen las teorías psicológicas y la evidencia empírica sobre la influencia de los afectos individuales en el proceso de toma de 3 Los estudios han mostrado relaciones positivas (Saunders et al. 1990; Knopf y Teall, 1996), convexas (Gorton y Rosen, 1995), cóncavas (Brewer y Saidenberg, 1996; Cebenoyan, Cooperman y Register, 1999) y negativas (Chen et al., 1998) entre la propiedad de los directivos y el risk taking. No obstante, estas relaciones contradictorias pueden ser el resultado de los diferentes períodos temporales y de las diferentes medidas de riesgo empleadas (Anderson y Fraser, 2000; Sullivan y Spong, 2005). En este sentido, Cebenoyan, Cooperman y Register (1995) y Anderson y Fraser (2000) encontraron diferentes asociaciones dependiendo del entorno regulatorio. 5 decisiones y en la asunción de riesgos. Estos planteamientos nos permiten desarrollar las hipótesis relativas a la influencia de los rasgos afectivos del CEO en el risk taking bancario. El cuarto apartado describe la muestra, las variables, y la metodología, el quinto muestra los resultados. El trabajo se cierra con la discusión de las principales conclusiones e implicaciones de nuestro estudio. 6 INTRODUCTION The analysis of determinants of banks’ risk taking behaviour has been one of the frequent concerns of banking literature. Setting aside market structure conditions4 or regulatory differences 5, empirical research has focused on several organizational and individual determinants of risk taking. Following Merton (1977) much of this research has based on the influence of fixed-rate deposit insurance systems on incentives of banks’ shareholders to increase the levels of risk taking- the moral hazard problem-. Deposit insurance can be considered as a put option like subsidy to shareholders, the value of which increases with bank risks, raising the incentives of shareholders to increase the levels of banks’ risk taking (Merton, 1977; Sharpe, 1978; Dothau and Williams, 1980). Other authors have considered the effect of organizational form and ownership structure on risk taking. Esty (1997a and b) found that stock thrifts exhibit greater risk than mutual thrifts and that converting from mutual to stock also increases risk taking. Barth, Hudson and Jahera (1995) and García Marco and Robles Fernandez (2002) obtained similar results for a sample of Texas savings and loans and Spanish banks and savings banks respectively. Schrand and Unal (1998) also found that mutual thrifts which convert to stock institutions increase total risk following conversion. Regarding the effect of ownership concentration, research by Laeven (2002) has show that risk is higher for banks with concentrated private ownership, especially those owned by a company or another financial institution and to a lesser extent in the case of those state or family owned. These and other related research show interesting explanations of determinants of risk taking but they have mainly focused on shareholder interests. Yet, it is management who takes the decisions that condition the risk structure of a bank. Following this argument, a second group of papers (Saunders, Strock and Travlos, 1990; Gorton and Rosen, 1995; Brewer and Saidenberg, 1996; Chen, Steiner and White, 1998; Cebenoyan, Cooperman and Register, 1999; Anderson and Fraser, 2000) have analyzed the role of managerial shareholdings on bank risk taking. These studies have obtained diverse relationships6, but all have found a significant 4 See Rhoades and Rutz (1982); Keeley (1990); Shy and Stenbacka (2004) and Brewer and Jackson (2006), among others. 5 See Dothau and Williams, 1980; Suárez (1998); Matutes and Vives (2000); Hendrickson and Nichols (2001); Barth, Caprio and Levine (2004) and González (2005), among others. 6 Studies have found positive (Saunders et al. 1990; Knopf and Teall, 1996), inverted U shaped (Gorton and Rosen, 1995), U shaped (Brewer and Saidenberg, 1996; Cebenoyan, Cooperman and Register, 1999) and negative (Chen et al., 1998) relationships between managerial shareholdings and risk taking. However, these conflicting relationships can be the results of differing time periods and measures of risk (Anderson and Fraser, 2000; Sullivan and Spong, 2005). In this sense, Cebenoyan, Cooperman and Register (1995) and Anderson and Fraser (2000) found different associations depending on the regulatory environment. 7 influence of managerial ownership on risk taking. These results show the influence of managerial decisions on the levels of bank risk taking. However, they have not focused on idiosyncratic differences across managers that can also influence on risk taking. During the last decades, several researchers on strategic management have been concerned with understanding how executive characteristics can condition their strategic choices, which in turn are reflected in organizational outcomes. This line of research, frequently termed as the “Upper Echelons” perspective, has focused on the role of certain demographic characteristics of CEOs (e.g., age, tenure, functional background, and formal education; Thomas et al., 1991; Hitt and Tyler, 1991; Jensen and Zajac, 2004) and psychological traits (e.g., risk aversion, locus of control, flexibility, and need for achievement; Gupta and Govindarajan, 1984; Miller et al., 1986) to explain organizational outcomes, such as risk-taking. Our study follows this line of research and incorporates a new psychological trait in “Upper Echelons” research: the role of CEO affective traits. The literature on emotions employs different terms, such as affect, mood, or emotion that are sometimes difficult to distinguish. We follow Forgas (1991) and other theorists who use “affect” as a general and inclusive label that refers to both emotion and mood. “Affective traits” refers to stable individual differences in the long-term tendencies to experience positive—e.g., excited, enthusiastic—or negative—e.g., guilty, irritable—affects (Rusting, 1998; Watson and Clark, 1984; Watson, Clark, and Tellegen, 1988). The remainder of our paper is structured as follows. The next section provides an overview of research that has shown how managerial characteristics do influence risk taking. Next we introduce the psychological theories and empirical evidence on the influence of affects on individuals’ decision making and risk taking, which allow us to develop hypotheses regarding the influence of CEO affective traits on bank risk taking. The third section describes the sample, the variables, and the methodology; the fourth shows the results. We close with a discussion of the main conclusions and implications of our study. DO CEO CHARACTERISTICS INFLUENCE ORGANIZATIONAL RISK TAKING? During the last decades, several researchers on strategic management have focused on understanding what conditions executives’ strategic choices, which in turn are reflected in organizational outcomes. “Upper-Echelons” research, developed by Hambrick and Mason (1984), has addressed the relationship between idiosyncratic differences of managers and risk taking in the strategic management literature. This perspective analyzes the relationships among managers’ demographic and background characteristics, their strategic choices, and 8 organizational outcomes. Specifically, it emphasizes how individual experiences shape executives’ cognition and values, which in turn affect their strategic choices (Hambrick and Mason, 1984; Wiersema and Bantel, 1992). Studies, mostly based on firms in non financial industries, have found significant effects on the adoption of risky strategic choices for observable characteristics such as age, tenure, functional background and formal education. Regarding age and tenure, research has followed Vroom and Pahl’s (1971) argument that managerial age is negatively related to risk-taking. Results obtained have shown that age and tenure are negatively associated to strategic choices that imply high levels of risk. Finkelstein and Hambrick (1990) found that top management team tenure was positively related to strategic persistence and conformity in strategy and performance. Wiersema and Bantel (1992) found that top executive tenure in the organization and age were negatively related to corporate strategic change. Similarly, research by Grimm and Smith (1991) showed that tenure in the industry and age of top executives in the railroad industry were inversely related to the degree that their firms changed strategies after deregulation. Finally, drawing on the typology of Miles and Snow (1978), Thomas, Litschert and Ramaswamy (1991) also found that executives of companies following defender strategies were older and had longer tenures in the company and in the position than those following prospector ones. “Upper-echelons” research has also related formal education and functional background experience to strategic choices involving risk taking. Research by Thomas et al. (1991) shows that executives of companies following prospector strategies had higher levels of educations than managers of defender ones. Geletkanycz and Black (2001) found that functional background diversity was positively related to commitment to the status quo. In summary, these results show that the adoption of strategies involving different levels of risk taking is also a consequence of differences in CEO characteristics. However, this line of research has traditionally ignored one factor in which individual decisions are commonly rooted: affects. The importance of affective factors in strategic decision-making has been recently asserted by authors such as Daniels (1998; 1999; 2003), Hodgkinson and Sparrow (2002), and Langley et al. (1995). Empirical research, although still scarce (Baldwin and Bengtsson, 2004; Daniels, 1998; Elsbach and Barr, 1999; Kisfalvi and Pitcher, 2003; Mittal and Ross, 1998; Staw and Barsade, 1993), has shown the influence of managers’ affects on their decisions and reveals the need to consider this topic both theoretically and empirically. 9 HYPOTHESES: IMPACT OF CEO AFFECTIVE TRAITS ON RISK TAKING Amongst the literature on the relationship between cognition and emotions, several theories have tackled the impact of emotions on decision making. One hypothesis common to these approaches is that of affective congruency (Rusting, 1998), which predicts that individuals better process information that is consistent with their affective state and their affective traits. The Affect as Information Model (Clore and Parrot, 1991; Schwarz and Bless, 1991; Schwarz and Clore, 1988) and the Network Theory of Affect (Bower, 1981, 1991) have addressed the influence of affective valence on selective perception, selective attention, learning and recall, and interpretations and associations. On the basis of the effects of affects on cognition researchers have analyzed the influence of affects on risk taking. In line with the congruency hypothesis, positive affect would lead to better recalling, attending, perceiving and interpreting positive information (Bower, 1981; Forgas, 1995). Therefore, when faced by any situation of a neutral, positive or even ambiguous nature, its evaluation by the individual with this affective valence will be positive (Isen and Shalker, 1982; Isen, Niedenthal and Cantor, 1992). In fact, research has shown that positive affect generates positive expectations (Isen, Shalker, Clark and Karp, 1978; Isen and Shalker, 1982), leads to overestimate the probabilities of positive events (Wright and Bower, 1992; Nygren et al, 1996) and decreases the estimated frequency of risks and other negative events (Johnson and Tversky, 1983; Wright and Bower, 1992; Nygren et al., 1996). Following these arguments, research has also shown that positive affects motivate decisions that are liable to be more risky, at least when facing a hypothetical situation (Isen and Patrick, 1983) or the chances of a meaningful loss are low for the decision maker (Arkes et al., 1988; Isen and Geva, 1987). Negative affects have been shown to increase estimates of the frequency of risks and other non desirable events, and to underestimate probabilities of positive events (Johnson and Tversky, 1983; Wright and Bower, 1992). As a consequence of these effects, Mano (1994) found that negative affectivity of subjects was related to more risk averse decisions. We therefore expected that positive affects of CEOs will be related to risk taking strategic choices, whereas negative affects will favour less risky decisions. H.1. Positive affective traits of CEOs will be positively related to risk taking. H.2. Negative affective traits of CEOs will be negatively related to risk taking. However, some empirical evidence has found opposite results (Isen and Patrick, 1983; Isen and Geva, 1987; Dunegan et al., 1992; Mittal and Ross, 1998). The explanation provided 10 by Isen and Patrick (1983), Isen and Geva (1987), Isen, Nygren and Ashby (1988) and Isen (2000) is based on the idea of affect maintenance. According to this idea, those subjects in a positive affect are motivated to maintain this affective state, for which reason they would not be ready to take risks. Similarly, it has been argued that, under negative affects, subjects would be willing to take greater risks in the hope that potential gains would alter their negative affects (Mittal and Ross, 1998). Under both arguments, Isen and colleagues consider two components that can be distinguished when analyzing risk preferences, which go in opposite direction: expected probability and utility. On the one hand, positive affect should increase the subjective positive probability, so that the CEO should categorize and interpret ambiguous information as an opportunity rather than as a threat (Dutton and Jackson, 1987; Jackson and Dutton, 1988; Mittal and Ross, 1998). On the other hand, however the negative utility for a potential loss should increase. However, the relevance of this second component can be questioned in the case of managers’ strategic choices (Mittal and Ross, 1998). It can be argued that the separation between ownership and control (Berle and Means, 1932; Jensen and Meckling, 1976) assures that potential losses for banks may not influence them personally, so that the motivational argument to maintain a positive affect may not exist. Nevertheless, these arguments lead us to treat the expected sign of the hypotheses with some caution. METHOD The hypotheses were tested in the Spanish banking industry. We sent out a survey in January 2004 to the CEOs of all Spanish banks and savings banks (70 banks and 46 savings banks), which contained a set of questions related to affective traits and demographic characteristics. The survey included a letter asking the CEOs to complete the questionnaire and promising anonymity. The questionnaire was pilot tested using two savings banks. Fifty-six questionnaires were returned, for a response rate of 48.3%, which we consider acceptable. From this sample, five questionnaires were discarded because the financial information needed for the analysis was not available. Two additional questionnaires were discarded since there was a CEO succession in the period of analysis. The final sample was composed entirely of men; average tenure was 25 years in the industry, and 10 as CEO. We examined and found no performance differences between institutions included in the sample and those excluded (p>0.10). Nonresponse bias related to CEO traits was investigated with the widely used method suggested by Armstrong and Overton (1977), which involves comparing early and al te respondents. This method is based on the finding that subjects who respond less readily are more like non- 11 respondents. Two sets of late respondents were defined corresponding to those who responded after receiving a follow-up call and the last 25 percent of the returned questionnaires. We found no differences between early and late respondents. To acquire financial data, we used the quarterly reports of banks and savings banks provided by the Asociación Española de Banca (Spanish Banking Association) and the Confederación Española de Cajas de Ahorros (Spanish Confederation of Savings Banks) for the period March 2003 December 20057. Independent variables CEOs’ affective traits were measured by a widely used scale, the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS), as developed by Watson et al. (1988), and adapted into Spanish by Sandín et al. (1999). The scale is composed of 20 items—10 items related to positive affects and the other half related to negative ones—and can be used for different time frames, from the present moment, which rates affective states, to a general option that rates affective traits; we employed the latter. Following Watson et al. (1988), we identified the categories of affect through a principal component analysis. Results showed a two-component solution, via complementary criteria: eigenvalue, the scree plot, and interpretability (Kim and Mueller, 1978). Component loadings for the varimax rotation are shown in Table 1. For the first component (labeled negative affect), nine negative affect items loaded; the remaining item—distressed—showed a loading of 0.292. The second component comprised positive affect items—interested, excited, strong, enthusiastic, proud, alert, inspired, determined, attentive, active—. Internal consistency reliabilities (Cronbach’s alpha) were 0.853 for negative affective traits and 0.820 for positive affective traits, above the 0.7 cut-off (Nunnally, 1978). INSERT TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE Control variables We included control variables related to CEO background and financial institutions’ characteristics. The background characteristics included in the questionnaire were those analyzed most frequently in “Upper-Echelons” research: tenure in the position, formal education, 7 Since all the CEOs of our sample had been in their position at least since 2002, we decided to begin our database in 2003. The survey measures demographic characteristics and affective traits -i.e., stable individual differences in the tendencies to experience positive and negative affects (Watson and Clark, 1984; Watson, Clark and Tellegen, 1988; Rusting, 1998)-, so that reverse causality was not expected to be a problem (Daniels, 1998; 1999). From our initial sample two institutions changed their CEOs during the period of analysis, so that our final sample included 49 CEOs. 12 and functional background diversity. Executives’ tenure has been negatively related to the willingness to take risk (Finkelstein and Hambrick, 1990). This relation has been evidenced in several firm outcomes. Executive tenure has been positively related to the commitment to the status quo (Hambrick, Geletkanycz and Fredrickson, 1993) and to performance conformity to the central tendencies of the industry (Finkelstein and Hambrick, 1990), and negatively related to strategic change (Grimm and Smith, 1991). Thomas et al. (1991) also found that executives of companies following defender strategies had longer tenures than prospector ones. Hambrick and Mason (1984) and Finkelstein and Hambrick (1996) have argued that education in management is negatively related to risk-taking and extreme performance. Our variable measured the level of formal education in management and economics, and ranged between 0 if CEOs had no formal education in the field and 4 if they had a PhD. Finally, we considered diversity in functional experience. Functional diversity can broaden managers’ belief structures (Walsh, 1988) and give them a wider array of strategic approaches, so that they perceive lower risks in alternatives that differ from central tendencies of the industry (Geletkanycz and Black, 2001). In order to measure functional experience diversity we followed Walsh (1988), who used Herfindahl's index formulation Y = ∑( X i 2 / X ) , in which Xi is the number of years of work in a given functional area i, and X is the number of years of work experience. Therefore, these measures show functional experience concentration. We used four characteristics of financial institutions as control variables. Size, measured as the log of total assets, had also been used in the analyses of Esty (1997a); Anderson and Fraser (2000), Konishi and Yasuda (2004) or Sullivan and Spong (2005) among others. The predicted effect on risk was negative since larger banks are more likely to have more diversified asset portfolios. Age, measured as the number of years since the financial institution was founded, had also been related to less risk taking (Esty, 1997a). The third control variable distinguished between banks and savings banks, by a dummy in which banks were rated as 1. Previous research by Esty (1997a and b), Barth et al. (1995); Schrand and Unal (1998), and García Marco and Robles Fernandez (2002) found that banks take more risks than savings banks. Location is a dummy variable which takes the value of 1 when the financial institution operates in a single province of Spain and 0 otherwise. This variable measures the possibilities of the institution to geographically diversify the risk (Saurina, 1998). Measures of bank risk To asses the level of risk of banks and savings banks we used two measures. The first measure is the variance in return on assets. Although a market based risk measure such as the variance 13 in stock returns is preferable, savings banks do not have traded equity. Nevertheless, it is a common measure of risk in finance and strategy research, (e.g. Cool, Dierickx and Jemison, 1989; Sinkey and Nash, 1993; Esty, 1997a; Palmer and Wiseman, 1999). The measure considered is the standard deviation of the return on assets over 12 quarters from 2003 to 2005. Following Nash and Sinkey (1997), we also considered the min/max difference of the 12 quarters to compute each bank’s and savings bank’s variability in ROA. Our second measure of risk is based on the “Z-score” developed by Hannan and Hanweck (1988). This measure has also been frequently employed in research on bank risk taking (Brewer, 1989; Boyd, Graham and Hewitt, 1993; Konishi and Yasuda, 2004). The z score is a measure of bank stability and indicates the distance from insolvency (Beck and Laeven, 2006). The empirical form of the “Z score” is: Z= [ROA+CAP]/s Were ROA = return on assets, CAP = is the capitalization ratio (equity capital to asset ratio), and s is the standard deviation of ROA. As stressed by Nash and Sinkey (1997), this measure of risk is appealing since it includes a widely used measure of bank’s performance (ROA), a common measure of risk in finance and strategy (variability of ROA), and the capitalization ratio. Thus Z is the number of standard deviations below the mean by which ROA must fall in order to eliminate equity. From this score, and using the Chebychev’s inequality, Hannan and Hanweck (1988) derive upper-bound probability of book-value insolvency: PU p=1/2(Z) 2 This measure of probability of insolvency has been employed as a measure of risk by Eisenbeis and Kwast (1991); Sinkey and Nash (1993); Garcia Marco and Robles Fernández (2002) and Blasko and Sinkey (2006), among others. Following our first measure of risk, the variability of ROA was computed both by the standard deviation and the min/max difference of ROA over the 12 quarters from 2003 to 2005. RESULTS Table 2 presents the means, standard deviations, and correlations of the variables. To test the hypotheses we used regression analyses. White’s (1980) test showed no problems of heteroskedasticity in all models. Values for the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) indicated no problems of collinearity. 14 INSERT TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE Results of the regression analyses taking as a dependent variable the probability of insolvency variable are shown in Table 3. Coefficients in this model show a significant association between CEOs’ affective traits and bank risk taking. Particularly, results show that negative affective traits are negatively related to the probability of insolvency (p<0.05). These results support hypothesis H2. In the case of positive affective traits, results show a no significant coefficients, so that hypothesis H1 is not supported. Regarding control variables, the positive and significant (p<0.05 and p<0.10) coefficients for management education suggest that CEOs with formal education in management are more likely to take more risky strategic choices for their institutions. The rest of the demographic variables show non-significant coefficients. Results also show a negative and significant sign for the size variable (p<0.05). This result is consistent with previous research (e.g., Esty, 1997a; Anderson and Fraser, 2000; Konishi and Yasuda, 2004; Sullivan and Spong, 2005) suggesting that larger financial institutions are more likely to have more diversified asset portfolios (Esty, 1997a). Finally, a financial institution’s age is negatively related to the probability of insolvency (p<0.05). The sign of the coefficient is consistent with that obtained by Esty (1997a), indicating that the younger the financial institution the more risk taking. The rest of the variables show nonsignificant coefficients. INSERT TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE Results of the regression analyses taking as a dependent variable the variability in ROA are shown in Table 4. Results obtained are very similar to models of probability of insolvency. The negative and significant (p<0.10) coefficients for negative affect suggests again that CEOs’ negative affects are related to less risk taking strategic choices, supporting H2. Coefficients for the positive affect variable were non-significant (p>0.10) not supporting H1. Several control variables are also found to be important determinants of risk taking. Specifically, results show a positive and significant coefficient for the management education variable in both models (p<0.05). In line with the analysis of probability of insolvency, models also show negative and significant coefficients for size (p<0.01) and age (p<0.10). INSERT TABLE 4 ABOUT HERE 15 DISCUSSION The aim of our study was to extend the research on determinants of bank risk taking by analyzing the impact of CEO affects. Much research has addressed the effect of financial institutions’ specific characteristics on their levels of risk. “Upper echelons” research has considered the influence of CEO background characteristics on the levels of risk taking of the firms they manage in non financial industries. Psychological literature has shown that affective valence influences on risk preference of people. Research by Mittal and Ross (1998) and Dunegan et al. (1992) have tested these findings in the context of organizational decisions. Our objective was to apply the arguments of this line of research to the banking literature. Our results reveal that CEOs’ affective traits do influence their strategic choices, which are reflected in risk taking. In particular, we found that negative affective traits condition less risk taking strategies, while positive affective traits have no effects on the levels of risk. These findings seem to support arguments by researchers on cognition and emotion that associate negative affects to increases in the estimates of the frequency of risks and other non desirable events (Johnson and Tversky, 1983; Daniels, 1998; Wright and Bower, 1992). Specifically, our results suggest that negative affects provide lower probabilities of positive events and overestimate probabilities of negative ones. In this sense, our results are very similar to those obtained by Mano (1994) in experiments based on students of a business school, who found that higher negative affectivity led to higher willingness to pay for insurance and lower negative affectivity led to greater willingness take risks. As stressed by Mano (1992; 1994) this result suggest negative affectivity prompts CEOs to frame the situation in more negative light and to protect themselves from further deterioration of their emotional state. Our findings also show effects of CEOs’ background on banks risk taking. CEOs with formal education in management and economics generated more risky strategic choices. This result contradicts the suppositions of Finkelstein and Hambrick (1996) and Hambrick and Mason (1984), who stressed that business school education teaches risk avoiding techniques. On the contrary, our findings are similar to those of Grimm and Smith (1991), who found that firms that changed their strategies were more likely to have MBAs among their Top Management Teams than were those that did not change them. Our results can also be related to research that has found positive associations between executive education levels and organization innovation (e.g., Bantel and Jackson, 1989; Thomas et al., 1991; Wiersema and Bantel, 1992). Since 16 innovation involves taking risk, they support our findings that those CEOs with higher management education tend to adopt more risk taking strategic choices. Finally, our results also suggest that differences in size and age also affect risk taking. These results are consistent with previous analyses suggesting that larger financial institutions are more likely to have more diversified asset portfolios (e.g., Esty, 1997a; Anderson and Fraser, 2000; Konishi and Yasuda, 2004; Sullivan and Spong, 2005) and that younger financial institutions take more risks (Esty, 1997a). The main implication of our research is that bank risk taking can be also conditioned by CEO affective traits and other background characteristics. In this sense our findings suggest opportunities to examine connections between background characteristics and psychological traits of CEOs and various other bank outcomes. It would also be interesting to extend the analysis to Top Management Teams, in line with research by Bantel and Jackson (1989) that has evidenced the relationship between the characteristics of Top Management Team members and innovation adoption in banks. We cannot conclude our paper without considering its main limitations. One of these is sample size. However, we consider that the response rate is appropriate for research of this nature. The sample analyzed represents 42 percent of the population, and we found no response bias related to CEO and financial intermediaries characteristics. Second, our analyses are based on one country in order to avoid the problem of allowing for different institutional constraints and regulations, but this choice limits at the same time the possibilities of generalizing our conclusions. We therefore think it would be especially interesting to extend the analysis to other countries to obtain new evidence of the effects of CEO affective traits. 17 Table 1. Factor loadings of the affect scale Factor 1 Negative affect Factor 2 Positive affect Interested Distressed Excited Upset Strong Guilty Scared Hostile Enthusiastic Proud Irritable Alert Ashamed Inspired Nervous Determined Attentive Jittery Active Afraid 0.033 0.292 0.206 0.793 0.017 0.672 0.730 0.756 0.257 0.347 0.637 -0.084 0.514 -0.160 0.609 -0.072 -0.133 0.677 -0.122 0.769 0.499 0.486 0.609 0.010 0.700 -0.202 -0.160 0.144 0.624 0.534 0.173 0.737 -0.032 0.515 0.089 0.688 0.619 0.170 0.700 -0.092 Eigenvalues Cumulative % of variance 4.660 23.302 4.340 45.004 18 Table 2. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations Means S.D. 1. Negative affect 0 1.000 1. 2. 3. 4. 2. Positive affect 0 1.000 0.000 3. Tenure 9.796 8.456 -0.315* 4. Management education 2.571 1.137 -0.095 0.035 0.346* 5. Functional background concentration 0.472 0.686 -0.074 -0.105 0.153 0.199 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. -0.048 6. Bank 0.469 0.504 0.052 -0.236 -0.089 -0.078 0.187 7. Size 15.392 1.686 -0.164 -0.014 0.189 0.255 -0.336* -0.189 8. Age 94.898 49.312 -0.123 0.168 0.227 0.313* -0.233 -0.151 9. Location 0.530** 0.837 0.373 0.001 -0.234 0.200 0.028 0.113 0.194 10. Probability of insolvency with standard deviation ROA 0.000 0.001 -0.208 -0.087 0.022 0.086 0.410** 0.293* 11. Probability of insolvency with min/max ROA 0.003 0.007 -0.223 -0.094 0.026 0.087 0.466** 0.287* -0.465** -0.428** 0.115 0.973** 12. Standard deviation ROA 0.001 0.003 -0.128 -0.073 0.051 0.077 0.215 0.248 -0.482** -0.394** 0.098 0.808** 0.665** 13. Min/max ROA 0.004 0.008 -0.138 -0.080 0.052 0.082 0.273 0.269 -0.517** -0.407** 0.102 0.848** 0.725** . *p<0.10; **p<0.05. 19 0.185 0.056 -0.485** -0.461** 0.116 0.993** Table 3. Regression analyses for probability of insolvency Model 1 Model 2 Probability of insolvency with standard deviation ROA Probability of insolvency with min/max ROA -0.306** 0.040 -0.309** 0.028 CEO background Tenure Management education Functional background concentration -0.050 0.268** 0.087 -0.065 0.230* 0.180 Financial intermediaries characteristics Bank Age Size Location 0.159 -0.339** -0.387** 0.177 0.147 -0.287** -0.351** 0.161 R2 F-ratio 0.4204 4.87*** 0.4036 4.61*** Model 3 Model 4 Independent variables CEO affective traits Negative affect Positive affect N=49. Standardized coefficients . *p<0.10; **p<0.05; ***p<0.01. Table 4. Regression analyses for ROA variation Standard deviation ROA Min/max ROA Independent variables CEO affective traits Negative affect Positive affect -0.225* 0.019 -0.236* 0.016 CEO background Tenure Management education Functional background concentration 0.036 0.296** -0.149 0.028 0.294** -0.096 0.144 -0.278* -0.511*** 0.186 0.151 -0.264* -0.535*** 0.188 0.2989 3.27*** 0.3529 3.91*** Financial intermediaries characteristics Bank Age Size Location R2 F-ratio N=49. Standardized coefficients . *p<0.10; **p<0.05; ***p<0.01. 20 REFERENCES Anderson RC, Fraser DR. 2000. Corporate control, bank risk taking, and the health of the banking industry. Journal of Banking and Finance 24: 1383-1398. Arkes HR, Herren LT, Isen AM. 1988. The role of potential loss in the influence of affect on risk-taking behaviour. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 47: 181-193. Baldwin ARG, Bengtsson M. 2004. The emotional base of interaction among competitors—an evaluative dimension of cognition. Scandinavian Journal of Management 20: 75-102. DOI: 10.1016/j.scaman.2004.05/004. Bantel KA, Jackson SE. 1989. Top management and innovation in banking: does the composition of the top team make a difference? Strategic Management Journal 10, special issue: 107-124. Barth JR, Caprio G, Levine R. 2004. Bank regulation and supervision: What works best? Journal of Financial Intermediation 13: 205-248. Barth JR, Hudson CD, Jahera JS. 1995. Risk-taking in the Texas S&L industry: Charter and ownership effects. The Financial Review 30: 115-137. Beck T, Laeven L. 2006. Resolution of failed banks by deposit insurers. Cross country evidence. World Bank Policy Research Paper 3920. Berle AA, Means GC. 1932. The Modern Corporation and Private Property. MacMillan Publishing, New York. Blasko M, Sinkey JF. 2006. Bank asset structure, real-estate lending, and risktaking. The Quarterly Review of Economics and Finance 46: 53-81. Bower GH. 1981. Mood and memory. American Psychologist 36: 129-148. Bower GH. 1991. Mood congruity of social judgments. In Emotion and Social Judgments, Forgas JP (ed). Pergamon: Elmsford NY; 31-53. Boyd JH, Graham SL, Hewitt S. 1993. Bank holding company mergers with nonblank financial firms: Effects on the risk of failure. Journal of Banking and Finance 17: 43-63. Brewer E. 1989. Relationship between bank holding company risk and nonblank activity. Journal of Economics and Business 41: 337-353. Brewer E, Jackson WE. 2006. A note on the “risk-adjusted” price-concentration relationship in banking. Journal of Banking and Finance 30: 1041-1054. Brewer E, Saidenberg, M. 1996. Franchise value, ownership structure, and risk at savings institutions. Federal Reserve Bank of New York Research Paper 9632. Cebenoyan AS, Cooperman ES, Register CA. 1995. Deregulation, reregulation, equity ownership, and S&L risk-taking. Financial Management 24(3): 63-76. Cebenoyan AS, Cooperman ES, Register CA. 1999. Ownership structure, charter value and risk-taking behavior of thrifts. Financial Management 28(1): 43-60. Chen C, Steiner T, Whyte A. 1998. Risk-taking behaviour and management ownership in depository institutions. The Journal of Financial Research 20: 146. 21 Clore GL, Parrott G. 1991. Moods and their vicissitudes: thoughts and feelings as information. In Emotion and Social Judgments, Forgas JP (ed). Pergamon: Elmsford NY; 107-123. Cool K, Dierickx I, Jemison D. 1989. Business strategy, market structure and risk-return relationships: A structural approach. Strategic Management Journal 10: 507-522. Daniels K. 1998. Towards integrating emotions into strategic management research: trait affect and perceptions of the strategic environment. British Journal of Management 9: 163-168. Daniels K. 1999. Affect and strategic decision making. Psychologist 12: 24-28. Daniels K. 2003. Asking a straightforward question: managers’ perceptions and managers’ emotions. British Journal of Management 14: 19-22. Dothau U, Williams J. 1980. Banks, bankruptcy and public regulation. Journal of Banking and Finance 4: 65-88. Dunegan KJ; Duchon D, Barton SL. 1992. Affect, risk, decision criticality: replication and extension in a business setting. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 53: 335-351. Dutton JE, Jackson SE. 1987. The categorization of strategic issues by decision makers and its links to organizational action. Academy of Management Review 12: 76-90. Eisenbeis RA, Kwast ML. 1991. Are real estate specializing depositories viable? Evidence from commercial banks. Journal of Financial Services Research 5: 524. Elsbach KD, Barr PS. 1999. The effects of mood on individuals’ use of structured decision protocols. Organization Science 10: 181-198. Esty BC. 1997a. Organizational form and risk taking in the savings and loan industry. Journal of Financial Economics 44: 25-55. Esty BC. 1997b. A case study of risk shifting in the savings and loan industry. Journal of Financial Economics 44 57-76. Finkelstein S, Hambrick DC. 1990. Top Management Team tenure and organizational outcomes: the moderating role of managerial discretion. Administrative Science Quarterly 35: 484-503. Finkelstein S, Hambrick DC. 1996. Strategic Leadership: Top Executives and Their Effects on Organizations. West Strategic Management Series. West: St. Paul MN. Forgas JP. 1991. Affect and social judgments: an introductory review. In Emotion and Social Judgments, Forgas JP (ed). Pergamon: Elmsford NY; 3-30. Forgas JP. 1995. Mood and judgment: the Affect Infusion Model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin 117: 39-66. García Marco T, Robles Fernández D. 2002. Estructura de propiedad y risk taking bancario: Un análisis empírico de cajas de ahorro y bancos españoles. XII Congreso Nacional de la Asociación Científica de Economía y Dirección de la Empresa, Palma de Mallorca. Geletkanycz MA, Black SS. 2001. Bound by the past? Experience-based effects on commitment to the strategic status quo. Journal of Management 27: 3-21. 22 González F. 2005. Bank regulation and risk-taking incentives: An international comparison of bank risk. Journal of Banking and Finance 29: 1153-1184. Gorton G, Rosen R. 1995. Corporate control, portfolio choice, and the decline of banking. The Journal of Finance I: 1377-1420. Grimm CM, Smith KG. 1991. Management and organizational change: a note on the railroad industry. Strategic Management Journal 12: 557-562. Gupta AK, Govindarajan V. 1984. Business unit strategy, managerial characteristics, and business unit effectiveness at strategy implementation. Academy of Management Journal 27: 25-41. Hambrick DC, Geletkanycz MA, Fredrickson JW. 1993. Top executive commitment to the status quo: some test of its determinants. Strategic Management Journal 14: 401-418. Hambrick DC, Mason PA. 1984. Upper echelons: the organization as a reflection of its top managers. Academy of Management Review 9: 193-206. Hannan TH, Hanweck GA. 1988. Bank insolvency risk and the market for large certificates of deposit. Journal of Money, Credit and Banking May: 203-211. Hendrickson JM, Nichols MW. 2001. How does regulation affect the risk taking of banks? A U.S. and Canadian perspective. Journal of Comparative Policy Analysis: Research and Practice 3: 59-83. Hitt MA, Tyler BB. 1991. Strategic decision models: integrating different perspectives. Strategic Management Journal 12(5): 327-351. Hodgkinson GP, Sparrow PR. 2002. The Competent Organization. Open University Press: Buckingham England. Hodgkinson GP. 2003. Editorial. British Journal of Management 14:1-2. Isen AM, Geva N. 1987. The influence of positive affect on acceptable level of risk: the person with a large canoe has a large worry. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 39: 145-154. Isen AS, Niedenthal P, Cantor N. 1992. An influence of positive affect on social categorization. Motivation and Emotion 16(1): 65-78. Isen AM, Patrick R. 1983. The effect of positive feelings on risk-taking: when the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 31: 194-202. Isen AM, Shalker TE, Clark M, Karp L. 1978. Affect, accessibility of material in memory and behavior: a cognitive loop? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36: 1-12. Isen AM, Shalker TE. 1982. Do you “accentuate the positive, eliminate the negative” when you are in a good mood? Social Psychology Quarterly 45: 345349. Isen AM. 2000: Positive affect and decision making. In Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM. (eds): Handbook of Emotions (2ªed.). The Guilford Press, New York; 417435. Isen AS, Nygren TE, Ashby FG. 1988. The influence of positive affect on the subjective utility of gains and losses: it is not worth the risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 55: 710-717. Jackson SE, Dutton JE. 1988. Discerning threats and opportunities. Administrative Science Quarterly 33: 370-387. 23 Jensen MC, Meckling WH. 1976. Theory of the firm: managerial behavior, agency cost, and ownership structure. Journal of Financial Economics 3: 305360. Jensen M, Zajac EJ. 2004. Corporate elites and corporate strategy: how demographic preferences and structural position shape the scope of the firm. Strategic Management Journal 25: 507-524. DOI: 10.1002/smj.393. Johnson E, Tversky A. 1983. Affect, generalization and the perception of risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45: 20-31. Keeley M. 1990. Deposit insurance, risk, and market power in banking. American Economic Review 80: 1184-1200. Kim J, Mueller CW. 1978. Factor Analysis. Statistical Methods and Practical Issues. Sage: Beverly Hills CA: Kisfalvi V, Pitcher P. 2003. Doing what feels right: the influence of CEO character and emotions on top management team dynamics. Journal of Management Inquiry 12: 42-66. DOI: 10.1177/1056492602250518. Knopf JD, Teall JL. 1996. Risk-taking behavior in the U.S. thrift industry: Ownership structure and regulatory changes. Journal of Banking and Finance 20: 1329-1350. Konishi M, Yasuda Y. 2004. Factors affecting bank risk taking: Evidence from Japan. Journal of Banking and Finance 28: 215-232. Laeven L. 2002. Bank risk and deposit insurance. The World Bank Economic Review 16: 109-137. Langley A, Mintzberg H, Pitcher P, Posada E, Saint-Macary J. 1995. Opening up the decision making: the view from the black stool. Organization Science 6: 260-279. Mano H. 1992. Judgment under distress: assessing the role of unpleasantness and arousal in judgment formation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 52: 216-245. Mano H. 1994. Risk-taking, framing effects, and affect. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 57: 38-58. Matutes C, Vives X. 2000. Imperfect competition, risk taking, and regulation in banking. European Economic Review 44: 1-34. Merton RC. 1977. An analytic derivation of the cost of deposit insurance and loan guarantees: An application of modern option pricing theory. Journal of Banking and Finance 1: 3-11. Miles RE, Snow CC. 1978. Organizational Strategy, Structure, and Process. McGraw-Hill: New York. Miller D, Kets de Vries MFR, Toulouse JM. 1986. Top executive locus of control and its relationship to strategy-making, structure, and environment. Academy of Management Journal 25: 237-253. Mittal V, Ross WT. 1998. The impact of positive and negative affect and issue framing on issue interpretation and risk taking. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 76: 298-324. Nash RC, Sinkey JF. 1997. On competition, risk, and hidden assets in the market for bank credit cards. Journal of Banking and Finance 21: 89-112. Nunnally JC. 1978. Psychometric Theory. McGraw-Hill: New York. 24 Nygren TE, Isen AM, Taylor PJ, Dulin J. 1996. The influence of positive affect on the decision rule in risk situations: focus on outcome (and especially avoidance of loss) rather than probability. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 66: 59-72. Palmer TB, Wiseman RM. 1999. Decoupling risk taking from income stream uncertainty: a holistic model of risk. Strategic Management Journal 20: 10371062. Rhoades SA, Rutz RD. 1982. Market power and firm risk: A test of the “quietlife” hypothesis. Journal Monetary Economics 9: 73-85. Rusting CL. 1998. Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of emotional information: three conceptual frameworks. Psychological Bulletin 124: 165-196. Sandin B, Chorot P, Lostao L, Joiner TE, Santed MA, Valiente RM. 1999. Escalas PANAS de afecto positivo y negativo: validación factorial y convergencia transcultural. Psicothema 11: 37-51. Saunders A, Strock E, Travlos NG. 1990. Ownership structure, deregulation, and bank risk taking. Journal of Finance 45: 643-654. Saurina J. 1998. Determinantes de la morosidad de las cajas de ahorros españolas. Investigaciones Económicas XXII: 393-426. Schrand CM, Unal H. 1998. Hedging and coordinated risk management: Evidence from Thrift conversions. Journal of Finance 53: 979-1013. Schwarz N, Bless H. 1991. Happy and mindless, but sad and smart? The impact of affective states on analytic reasoning. In Emotion and Social Judgments, Forgas JP (ed). Pergamon: Elmsford NY; 55-71. Schwarz N, Clore GL. 1988. How do I feel about it? The informative function of affective states. In Affect, Cognition, and Social Behavior, Fiedler K, Forgas JP (eds). Hogrefe: Toronto; 44-62. Sharpe WF.1978. Bank capital adequacy, deposit insurance and security values. Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis 13: 701-718. Shy O, Stenbacka R. 2004. Market structure and risk taking in the banking industry. Journal of Economics 82(3): 249-280. Sinkey JF, Nash RC. 1993. Assessing the riskiness and profitability of creditcard banks. Journal of Financial Services Research 2: 127-150. Staw BM, Barsade S. 1993. Affect and managerial performance: a test of the sadder-but-wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypotheses. Administrative Science Quarterly 38: 304-331. Suárez J. 1998. Risk taking and the prudential regulation of banks. Investigaciones Económicas XXII(3), 307-336. Sullivan RJ, Spong K. 2005. Managerial wealth, ownership structure, and risk in commercial banks. SSRN working paper. http://ssrn.com/abstract=558684 Thomas AS, Litschert RJ, Ramaswamy K. 1991. The performance impact of strategy-manager coaligment: an empirical examination. Strategic Management Journal 12: 509-522. Vroom VH, Pahl B. 1971. Relationship between age and risk-taking among managers. Journal of Applied Psychology 55: 399-405. 25 Walsh JP. 1988. Selectivity and selective perception: an investigation of managers’ belief structures and information processing. Academy of Management Journal 31: 873-896. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. 1988. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54: 1063-1070. Watson D, Clark LA. 1984. Negative affectivity: The disposition in experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin 96: 465-490. White H. 1980. A heteroskedasticity-consistent covariance matrix estimator and a direct test for heteroskedasticity. Econometrica 48: 817-838. Wiersema MF, Bantel KA. 1992. Top management team demography and corporate strategic change. Academy of Management Journal 35: 91-121. Wright WF, Bower GH. 1992. Mood effects on subjective probability assessments. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 52: 276-291. 26