Subido por Alberto Mario Ceballos Arroyo

Norell...JBO1983BehavioralsensitivityAgriEng

Anuncio
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/275578859
Behavioral Studies of Dairy Cattle Sensitivity to Electrical Currents
Article in Transactions of the ASAE · January 1983
DOI: 10.13031/2013.34160
CITATIONS
READS
27
87
4 authors, including:
Richard John Norell
J. Bruce Overmier
University of Idaho
University of Minnesota Twin Cities
10 PUBLICATIONS 92 CITATIONS
184 PUBLICATIONS 5,085 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
Just finished a review paper on Learned Helplessness. View project
All content following this page was uploaded by J. Bruce Overmier on 16 November 2015.
The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
SEE PROFILE
Behavioral Studies of Dairy Cattle
Sensitivity to Electrical Currents
R. J. Norell, R. J. Gustafson, R. D. Appleman, J. B. Overmier
MEMBER
ASAE
ABSTRACT
LECTRICAL resistance data were collected for eight
pathways through dairy cows. Significant variation in
resistance was found between pathways through
individual cows and between cows. The mean path
resistances ranged from 359 ohms for a mouth-all hooves
pathway to 738 ohms for a front-rear hooves pathway.
The distribution for the mouth-all hooves pathway
showed 25% of the population below 302 ohms and 75%
below 441 ohms. Three experiments assessing animal
sensitivity to current based on behavioral indicators were
performed. No suppression of a learned response to
obtain food was found up to 6.0 mA front-rear hooves
shock. However, muzzle-all hooves shocks as low as 1.0
mA suppressed plate pressing behavior. A specific
avoidance response to a mouth-all hooves shock was
exhibited 13.8% of the time at 1.0 mA and 92.3% at 4.0
mA, while a learned escape response to a front-rear
hooves shock above a normal activity level occurred
between 2.0 and 3.0 mA.
E
INTRODUCTION
Awareness of the presence of stray voltage in livestock
facilities has risen rapidly in the last few years. However,
very limited data are available on the electrical
characteristics and current sensitivity of farm animals.
The first component of this paper reports data collected
on the resistance to alternating current (ac, 60 Hz) for
likely pathways through dairy cows. The second
component reports results of three experiments
measuring sensitivity to electrical currents based on
behavioral indicators.
The specific objectives of the resistance measurement
component of this study were to: (a) measure resistance
of eight pathways through dairy cows, (b) determine
correlations bewteen cow attributes (body weight, wither
height, body length and age) and measured pathway
resistances, and (c) estimate the contribution of the
hooves to the total path resistance.
The objective of the current sensitivity component of
this study was to study cow response to graduated levels
Article was submitted for publication in January, 1983; reviewed and
approved for publication by the Electric Power and Processing Div. of
ASAE, 1983. Presented as ASAE Paper No. 82-3530.
Published as Paper No. 13,214 of the scientific journal series of the
Minnesota Agricultural Experiment Station.
The authors are: R. J. NORELL, Assistant Professor and Extension
Dairyman, Animal Science Dept., University of Idaho, Idaho Falls; R.
J. GUSTAFSON, Associate Professor, Agricultural Engineering Dept.,
and R. D. APPLEMAN, Professor and Extension Dairyman, Animal
Science Dept., J. B. OVERMIER, Professor, Psychology, University of
Minnesota, St. Paul.
Acknowledgment: The authors wish to acknowledge support
received from the National Rural Electric Cooperative Assn. for stray
voltage research at the University of Minnesota.
1506
of mouth-all hooves and front-rear hooves currents. The
criteria for assessing cow response were behavioral
indices. In one experiment, shocks were an aversive
stimulus to a learned food acquiring response. The
indicator was a partial suppression or slowing down of
the learned response.
In a second experimental approach, response
functions to random front-rear hooves and mouth-all
hooves shocks were determined. The shock were random
in that the initiation of the shock was not under the cow's
control as in experiment one. The response indices were
the displaying of a learned escape to a front-hooves shock
and a shock elicited mouth opening due to a mouth-all
hooves shock.
REVIEW OF LITERATURE
Resistance variability between cows and pathways is
clearly evident from the available data. Phillips and
Parkinson (1963), Craine et al. (1970), Woolford (1972),
Whittlestone et al. (1975) and Lefcourt (1982) reported
average resistances in the range of 250 to 1200 ohms for
various pathways. A combination of differences in
method of measurement, contact resistances, and actual
path resistances likely explain the five-fold or more
differences in resistances between specific pathways.
Altered animal behavior and milking characteristics,
increased incidence of mastitis, and lowered milk
production have been reported as a consequence of
electric shocks (Fairbank and Craine, 1978; Cloud et al.,
1981; Surbrook and Reese, 1981; Williams, 1981). The
relationship between reported cow responses and shock
intensity are poorly understood and have received limited
study. The majority of the existing research involved the
application of shocks during the act of milking. Shocks
from the milking machine had a minimal effect on milk
flow characteristics or milking times (Phillips and
Parkinson, 1963; Woolford and Copeland, 1969; and
Woolford, 1972). However, in the latter two studies,
cattle were reluctant to enter the parlor and exhibited
signs of annoyance such as increased hoof movement.
Woolford (1972) concluded that voltage gradients along
the parlor floor or between the parlor pipework and floor
may be more bothersome to the cow than shocks from
the milking machine.
Whittlestone et al. (1975) obtained threshold
responses for four animal pathways likely to be exposed
to shocks in the milking parlor. The body pathways
studied were: one teat-all hooves; all teats-all hooves;
rump-all hooves; and chest-all hooves. The threshold
response was a change in preference of a paddle pressed
for a grain reward. One paddle turned the current "on"
and the other turned it "off. Average threshold voltages
and currents were: teat-all hooves, 6.2 V/7.1 mA; all
tests-all hooves, 7.8 V/5.6 mA; rump-all hooves, 4.3
V/6.1 mA; and chest-all hooves, 4.0 V/4.0 mA.
© 1983 American Society of Agricultural Engineers 0001-2351/83/2605-1506$02.00
TRANSACTIONS of the ASAE—1983
VOLTMETER I
STEP DOWN
ISOLATING
TRANSFORMER
STEP DOWN
AUTOTRANSFORMER
r ~
1
TABLE 1. AVERAGE PATHWAY RESISTANCE MEASURED UNDER DRY
AND WET HOOF-GRID CONTACT CONDITIONS, OUTLIER REMOVED
(n = 10 JERSEYS).
Condition
X
SD
X
SD
1562
1265
621
(470)
(378)
(191)
1479
1167
647
(275)
(321)
(173)
FIXED RESISTANCE
R= 79 kXi
Fig. 1—Electrical resistance measurement circuit.
Lefcourt (1982) studied cow response to graded shock
increments applied to a front-rear leg pathway.
Electrodes were attached above one front and one rear
hoof. Average currents and voltages corresponding to a
milk and strong response were: milk, 0.8 V/2.5 mA;
strong, 1.1 V/3.5 mA.
Henke et al. (1982) studied the response of four cows
to udder-hooves shock. An electrode was attached at the
back of the udder beneath the tail while the animal stood
on perforated steel. Behavioral responses occurred at
currents of between 2 mA and 4 mA. Endocrine
responses were poorly correlated with shock intensity.
RESISTANCE MEASUREMENTS
Equipment and Procedure
The floor of a wood-frame tie stall was adapted to
make separate contacts with front and rear hooves. Two
expanded metal grates (0.8 m x 0.9 m) isolated by a
section of cow mat and rubber matting were placed on
the stall floor. Other contact points were established by
(a) a steel bit in the cow's mouth and (b) an aluminum
foil-lined teat cup inflation (plus EKG paste) attached to
the left front teat. Eight different circuits or pathways
through the cow were defined using these connections.
The electrical circuit used for resistance measurements
(Fig. 1) was designed to maintain a constant current. A
fixed impedance source as described by Masterton and
Campbell (1967) was used. With this system, normal
variation in cow resistance was expected to produce less
than a 1.5% change in current level.
Digital volumeters were used to measure voltage across
the fixed resistor and cow pathway. The current flow
through the circuit was adjusted to 0.1 mA. Calculated
cow pathway resistance was recorded as the average of
five determinations. Standard errors of the mean were
small for the five determinations, usually 5-10 ohms.
Results
Experiment 1R—Dry and Wet Hooves Comparisons
The objective of this experiment was to compare the
influence of wet versus dry hooves on pathway resistance.
The hypothesis was that a wet hoof would lower the hoofgrid contact resistance and thereby decrease the overall
pathway resistance. Data were collected in two different
sessions. In one session, the underside of the hoof was
brushed to remove dry manure and bedding. This was
the dry hoof condition. In the other session, the hooves
were carefully washed and the metal dampened before
resistance measurement. The grids and hooves were
sprayed with water between determinations to maintain
the wet hoof conditions.
1983—TRANSACTIONS of the ASAE
Mean
difference
(dry-wet)
Standard
error
83
98
-26
101
95
16
,
Front-rear hooves
Mouth-front hooves
Mouth-rear hooves
VOLTMETER 2
Wet
Dry
-V\A/
The resistance of a front-rear hooves, mouth-front
hooves, and mouth-rear hooves pathway were
determined, under wet and dry hoof conditions, on
eleven Jersey cows. Statistical comparisons were paired
t-tests of pathway resistance measured under wet and dry
hoof conditions.
Upon analyzing different pairs (dry-wet for an
individual cow), it was found that data for one cow
accounted for the large inequality in variance between
hoof conditions. The reasons for the greater dry hoof
resistance of this outlier cow are not known. Data from
this cow indicated pathway resistance may be
considerably increased under dry hoof conditions.
Deleting these data from the analyses nearly halved the
pathway resistance variance between cows under the dry
hoof measurement conditions. Average pathway
resistances and differences between dry and wet
conditions for the reduced data set are in Table 1.
Variances in pathway resistance between measurement
conditions were not significantly different in the reduced
data set. Mean differences between wet and dry hoof
resistances on the expanded metal grids were also not
significant.
Experiment 2R—Resistance of Eight Pathways
The electrical resistance of eight defined cow pathways
were measured on twenty-eight Holstein cows.
Resistance determinations were made under the "wet
hoof-grid" contact conditions as defined for Experiment
1R. Pathway resistances were compared with a
randomized block statistical design. Cows were the
blocks and pathways were the treatments. A log 10
transformation was used to make the pathway resistance
data more normal and to reduce variance inequality.
Mean pathway resistances and their associated
standard deviations are presented in Table 2. Ranking
the measured pathways from lowest to highest resistance
yields: (a) mouth-all hooves; (b) mouth-teat; (c) mouthrear hooves; (d) teat-all hooves; (e) mouth-front hooves;
(f) teat-rear hooves; (g) front-rear hooves; and (h) teatfront hooves.
TABLE 2. MEAN RESISTANCES AND STANDARD
DEVIATIONS FOR THE EIGHT MEASURED COW PATHWAYS
(n = 28 HOLSTEINS).
Log 10 ohms
Pathway
X
SD
Front-rear hooves
Mouth-front hooves
Mouth-rear hooves
Mouth-all hooves
Mouth-teat
Teat-front hooves
Teat-rear hooves
Teat-all hooves
2.868
2.789
2.686
2.555
2.656
2.944
2.861
2.778
.130
.109
.129
.106
.149
.133
.137
.141
Geometric
mean, ohms
738
615
485
359
452
877
726
600
1507
TABLE 3. RESISTANCE DISTRIBUTIONS BY BODY
PATHWAY (n = 28 HOLSTEINS).
TABLE 4. CONTRIBUTION OF THE HOOF AND HOCK TO THE TOTAL
PATHWAY RESISTANCE (n • 9 HOLSTEINS).
vleasuremenl ' (ohms)
Percentiles*
10%
Front-rear hooves
Mouth-front hooves
Mouth-rear hooves
Mouth-all hooves
Mouth-teat
Teat-front hooves
Teat-reat hooves
Teat-all hooves
25%
50%
A
75%
90%
Cow pathway
Front-rear hooves
713
Mouth-front hooves
Mouth-rear hooves
Mouth-all hooves
652
596
437
496
590
734
875
1152
420
345
244
541
398
302
624
475
361
715
572
441
851
776
525
294
354
433
597
713
593
503
402
679
594
455
874
710
594
1048
1508
1203
894
767
Significant differences existed between the pathway
means. Constrasts were used to compare pathway
resistances including: two versus four hooves; front
versus rear hooves; and mouth versus the teat. Pathway
resistances including four hooves were significantly lower
than those including two hooves. The resistance of
pathways including front hooves only were greater than
those including rear hooves only. Resistances of both
pathways including mouth-hoof combinations were lower
the those including teat-hoof combinations. The mouthteat pathway was significantly lower in resistance than
the teat-hoof pathway combinations. The front-rear
hooves pathway resistance was larger than the mouthhoof combinations but not the teat-hoof combinations.
Variability between cows also exists for a given
pathway. Selected percentile limits (10%, 25%, 50%,
75%, and 90%) for each pathway are displayed in Table
3. A percentile defines the percent of the population
below the listed limit. These data are useful in
illustrating differences in current flow between cows due
to an applied voltage. For example, assuming a mouthall hooves pathway,
10%
= 244
ohms and Rgo% = 525 ohms.
In this case, 10% of the cattle exposed to a 1.0 V mouthall hooves shock would receive a 4.0 mA or greater shock
while 90% of the cattle would receive a 1.9 mA or greater
shock. These data demonstrate a two-fold difference in
resistance and resulting current flow within the middle
80% of the population.
Simple correlations between individual pathway
resistances and indices of cow size and age were
calculated. Selected indices of cow were: body weight
(kg), heart girth (cm), wither height (cm), body length
(withers to pins, cm) and age (mo). There were seven
cows each in their first through fourth lactations.
Correlations between body measurements and
pathway resistances were inconclusive. Pathway
resistances were not correlated with body weight, heart
girth, and body length. Wither height was significantly
correlated with teat-front hooves (r = 0.38) and teat-all
hooves (r = 0.37) resistance. Age was negatively
correlated (r = -0.45) with the resistance of the mouthrear hooves and mouth-all hooves pathways. The
repeatability of this age-pathways resistance correlation
should be examined. Numbers were relatively small
within age categories.
1508
B
SD
Difference?
(A-B)
Standard
error of
difference
X
SD
327
(97)
386*
105
292
265
197
(52)
(31)
(16)
360*
331*
240*
49
64
34
* (p < . 0 1 , paired t-test)
' Measurement A included the hooves and hocks in the cow pathway. Measurement
B was the pathway terminating at EKG electrodes attached above the hock on each
hoof.
+ The difference between measurements A and B was assumed to be the hoof and
hock contribution to total pathway resistance.
953
* The percentiles are the percent of the measured resistances below
the reported limit.
R
X
ill!
Pathway
Experiment 3R—Hoof and Hock Resistance
The objective of this trail was to estimate the
contribution of the hoof and hock to the total pathway
resistance. The hoof contributes to the total pathway by
its internal resistance and by the hoof-grid contact
resistance. Since direct measure was not possible, an
estimate was derived by measuring two pathways
through the cow. One path involved the mouth-all hooves
pathway. The second path involved the mouth to EKG
electrodes plus paste applied above the hocks. This
configuration removed the hooves from the circuit. The
difference between the two paths provides an estimate of
the resistance of the four hooves in parallel. This
estimate is only an approximation of hoof resistance
because: (a) the electrode alters the current flow pattern
through the leg and (b) the electrode-skin contact has an
associated contact resistance which is not a part of the
internal body resistance.
Data were measured (with and without the hooves) for
four pathways using nine Holstein cows. Average
pathway resistances, with and without hooves, are
displayed in Table 4. Total pathway resistance was
decreased by approximately 55% for all four measured
circuits when the hooves were not included.
BEHAVIORAL EXPERIMENTS
Experiment IB—Aversion to Learned Response
Equipment and Procedure
A specially constructed the stall was used for this
experiment. The stall base contained two separate basins
(0.8 m x 0.9 m) with expanded metal floors for contact
with the front and rear hooves. Water was added to the
basins to assure a stable hoof-grid contact.
STEP DOWN
AUTOTRANSFORMER
120 V
STEP UP
(112)
TRANSFORMER
FIXED
RESISTANCE
-AAA,
RF = 28 k &
SHOCK
CONTROL
RELAY
CONTACT
1|
Re
IHTTH
FRONT AND REAR GRIDS
PRESS
PLATE
SWITCH
*
24 V
*
IMPULSE COUNTER
AND RECORDER
SHOCK CONTROL
AND TIMER
Fig. 2—Electrical circuitry for Experiment IB.
TRANSACTIONS of the ASAE—1983
T A B L E 5. P L A T E P R E S S I N G S U P P R E S S I O N T H R E S H O L D S
The schematic of the electrical circuit is illustrated in
DUE TO MUZZLE-ALL HOOVES SHOCKS.
Fig. 2. The application of the shock was contingent upon
a muzzle plate press. The first treatment, front-rear
Session
hooves shocks, was initiated by a microswitch located
1
2
3
inside the press plate. The second treatment, muzzle-all Cow
A*
D*
A*
D*
A*
D*
hooves shocks, was initiated by touching the metal plate.
milliamps
In other words, the plate was "live" and the cow
completed the circuit by touching it. The maximum
952
1.0
3.5
1.0
899
1.0
1.0
2.0
deliverable current to the cow from this source was 6.0
967
2.0
2.0
2.0
mA. Current flows were within 2% of the desired value
ND1"
4.5
968
ND1"
when a cow was placed in the circuit.
1013
4.5
4.0
4.5
1.0
1.0
3.0
A metal press plate and the grain delivery system was 1019
positioned in front of the cow. Six Holsteins were trained * S h o c k series, A stands for ascending and D for descending series.
to press the plate by the method of sequential ' N D = n o t d e t e r m i n e d , an ascending series of 0 . 2 5 m A i n c r e m e n t s
u p t o 4.0 m A was p r e s e n t e d .
approximation. Initially, the experimenter manually + NM
= n o t m e a s u r e d , cow w o u l d n o t stabilize h e r plate pressing
activated the feeder for head movements close to the
behavior.
press plate. Secondly, grain rewards were given only
when the cow's muzzle was in front of the press plate.
The cattle would begin plate pressing shortly after they avoidance of the "live" metal plate. The cow would make
had reached the second stage of training. Grain rewards several touch contacts before pressing the plate with
were delivered upon each plate press for three sessions sufficient force to activate the plate press recorder. Once
(each 0.5 h duration). The grain rewards were 70 grams started, plate pressing was conducted at the normal rate.
of a mixture of beet pulp (75%) and shelled corn (25%). The interval between grain rewards was lengthened due
The number of plate presses required to receive grain to these touch avoidances.
The shock intensities which suppressed plate pressing
rewards was gradually increased (in five-press
increments) until stable rates were achieved at 30 presses behavior are shown in Table 5. Suppression currents
per reward. Based on Azrin and Holz (1959), the plate varied between cows and sessions. Four out of six cows
press contingent shocks in this study were expected to were initially suppressed by 1.0 to 2.0 mA shocks. Higher
lengthen the interval between grain rewards. A grain shock intensities were required to suppress plate pressing
reward interval was the time interval starting at the behavior during later sessions. The reasons for the
initiation of plate pressing and ending at the start of adaptation by these cows by 1.0 to 2.0 mA are not
plate pressing for the next reward. The lengthening of known. Shocks of 4 to 4.5 mA were required to initially
this interval is a slowing down or partial suppression of suppress plate pressing behavior of one other cow.
Response suppression then continued for two consecutive
the learned food acquiring response.
0.5
mA decrements.
A one-tailed 95% confidence limit for the mean of
A
definitive low suppression threshold cannot be
three grain reward intervals was the response threshold
measure. Data used to calculate the 95% one-tailed limit derived from these data. The touch avoidances were
were the mean grain reward interval and standard occurring over a broad range of shocks (1.0 to 4.5 mA).
deviation from the last three control plate pressing days. Suppression currents were generally between 3.0 and 4.5
Approximately 150 grain reward intervals were measured mA on the second session (4 to 6 cows) and third session
(5 of 5 cows).
on these control days for each cow.
The treatments were front-rear hooves and muzzle-all
hooves shocks. Three separated experimental sessions Experiment 2B—Learned Escape Responses,
were conducted for each treatment. During a session, a Front-Rear Hooves Shock
cow was alloted four hundred fifty no-shock plate presses Equipment and Procedure
A 1.5 m high wall was built around the stall described
to "warm up". An ascending series of shocks (0.5 mA
in
Experiment IB for use in this trial. Electrical
increments) was then presented. Cattle were exposed to
each shock intensity for three grain reward intervals until connections to the animal were the two metal grids in
the 95% confidence limit was exceeded. If the limit was water-filled basins. A manually operated switch,
exceeded, the cow's plate pressing behavior was controlled by an observer, initiated the shock. A second
considered suppressed. A decending series followed (0.5 operator adjusted the electrical current level, such that
mA decrements) until the mean of three grain reward the animal observer was unaware of the current level.
Seven Holstein cows were exposed to fifty training
intervals was within the confidence limit.
shocks during one session. A 5.0 mA front-rear hooves
pulsating (repeated on and off at approximately 60 cycles
Results
per minute) shock was used. The maximum training
Front-rear hooves shocks up to the 6 mA limit did not shock duration was thirty seconds. The shocks were
suppress plate pressing behavior. Apparently, higher terminated early when the cow picked up a front hoof.
shock intensities through this body pathway are required The interval between shocks was variable, ranging from
to influence plate pressing behavior. Subjective signs of thirty to ninety seconds. All seven cows were picking up a
annoyance such as hoof movement during plate pressing front hoof on 90 to 100% of the trials at the conclusion of
were occasionally observed. However, the observed hoof the training. The learned escape response was expressed
movement occurred too infrequently to correlate with in less than 10 seconds from the shock onset.
shock intensity.
During a second session, twenty-four hours later, the
Plate pressing behavior was suppressed by muzzle-all cattle were exposed to twenty 5.0 mA "warm-up" shocks
hooves shocks. The typical response was a touch followed by a series of sixty shocks. Front hoof pickups
1983—TRANSACTIONS of the ASAE
1509
T A B L E 6. O B S E R V E D P E R C E N T ESCAPES BY C U R R E N T
I N T E N S I T Y F O R F R O N T - R E A R H O O V E S SHOCKS (n = 6 0 ) .
Current, m A
0.0
1.0
2.0
3.0
4.0
5.0
Escapes,
18.3
23.2
25.0
43.3
71.7
96.7
Difference, '
SE,%
5.0
1.7
18.3*
28.4**
25.0**
6.8
6.8
8.0
8.5
6.5
9 5 % CONFIDENCE
INTERVAL
* (p<0.05)
**(p<0.01)
occurred on 90 to 100% of the warm-up trials. The shock
series, which followed the warm-up trials, included six
current intensities (0.0, 1.0, 2.0, 3.0, 4.0, and 5.0 mA)
which were randomly assigned within ten blocks of time.
Two cows were exposed to only five replications of each
intensity. The pulsating shocks were presented for a
maximum duration of 10 seconds. A front hoof pickup
terminated the shock trial early. Response rates
(summed across cows) were compared between adjacent
1.0 mA pairs with a chi square test of proportion in
paried samples.
Results
The observed escape frequency (in percent) for each
current intensity is shown in Table 6. The observed
twenty percent escape rate at the lower three intensity is
assumed to be random front hoof movement rather than
an attempt to escape the shock. This was verified by
preliminary study, where under no shock conditions ten
cows were observed to pick up a front hoof on twenty-two
percent of the observation trials (n = 50 per cow).
Significant increases in escape percentage occurred for
each 1.0 mA increment above 2.0 mA. The cattle were
expressing the learned escape response to avoid shocks
above 2.0 mA. The response rate was 96.7% at 5.0 mA.
An escape function was calculated by logit regression
analysis for the 2.0 to 5.0 mA shock range. The escape
function and its associated 95% confidence interval are
shown in Fig. 3.
Experiment 3B—Learned Escape Response,
Mouth-All Hooves Shock
Equipment and Procedure
Using the same facility as Experiment 2B, seven cows
were subjected to mouth-all hooves shocks. A
characteristic response of opening the mouth was
observed for electrical shock via this path. For this
experiment, the cattle were trained by ten "warm-up"
trials of 5 mA. The current was terminated when the cow
displayed the mouth opening response. This response
occurred on 90% of the warm-up trials in less than 10
seconds. The cattle were then exposed to the same series
of sixty shocks as in the previous experiment.
-V
2.0
Current, m A
0.0
1.0
2.0
3.0
4.0
5.0
** p < 0 . 0 1
1510
Response,
0.0
13.8
30.0
69.2
92.3
98.4
Difference,
13.8**
16.2**
39.2**
23.1**
6.1
SE, <
4.3
5.7
6.5
6.2
4.5
4.0
5.0
Fig. 3—Response rate vs. current for front-rear hooves
shock—Experiment 2B.
The statistical comparison procedures were identical
with Experiment 2B. Only the first five replicates of each
shock intensity were used for one cow. She altered her
response from a mouth opening response in the first five
replicates to a hoof movement response in the second set
of five replicates. The reason for the change in response
is not known.
Results
The observed response frequencies to mouth-all
hooves shocks are presented in Table 7. Significant
increases in response rates occurred between each 1.0
mA increment comparison up to the 4.0 mA. Cow
response averaged less than 15% at 1.0 mA and
exceeded 90% at 4.0 and 5.0 mA. No responses were
observed during the control no-shock trials. This result
indicates the mouth opening was a specific shockavoidance response.
A response function to mouth-all hooves shocks was
calculated by logit regression. The function and its
associated 95% confidence interval are displayed in Fig.
4. The function shows the variation in response rate by
current level. For example, a 2.5 mA current level would
yield a 50% response rate.
—
- :^
_
-
/ / ^
#
-
///
///
T A B L E 7. O B S E R V E D P E R C E N T R E S P O N S E BY C U R R E N T
L E V E L F O R M O U T H - A L L H O O V E S SHOCKS (n = 6 5 ) .
3.0
CURRENT! MILLIAMPERES)
irK
/
p-
/ S L
/ / /
.,
95 % CONFIDENCE
INTERVAL
///
*-
V/
- ^ .y #
•— i —
1
1
1
2-0
1
1
3.0
1
1
4.0
1
1
CURRENT ( MILLIAMPERES)
Fig. 4—Response rate vs. current for mouth-all hooves
shock—Experiment 3B.
T R A N S A C T I O N S of the ASAE—1983
6.0|—
FRONT-REAR HOOVES SHOCK
POPULATION RESISTANCE PERCENTILES
MOUTH-ALL HOOVES SHOCK
POPULATION RESISTANCE PERCENTILES
4.0
\-
o
9 2.0
^°'»
1.0
20
40
60
80
100
RESPONSE RATE (%)
Fig. 5—Voltage vs. response rate for mouth-all hooves
shock.
Fig. 6—Voltage vs. response rate for front-rear hooves
shock.
SUMMARY AND INTERPRETATION
Significant variation in resistance was found between
pathways through individual cows and between cows.
The lowest resistance appeared to be the mouth-all
hooves pathway with a mean of 359 ohms. The
distribution for this pathway showed 25% of the
population below 302 ohms and 75% below 441 ohms.
The hoof resistance was found to be a large component
of path resistance. However, correlation of resistances
with body parameters was inconclusive. Both of these
areas need further investigation.
In the behavioral experiment phase, no suppression of
a learned response to obtain feed was found for up to a
6.0 mA front-rear hooves shock. However, muzzle-all
hooves shocks as low as 1.0 mA suppressed plate
pressing behavior.
A specific avoidance response to a mouth-all hooves
shock was exhibited 13.8% of the time at 1.0 mA and
92.3% at 4.0 mA, while a learned escape response to a
front-rear hooves shock above a normal activity level
occurred between 2.0 and 3.0 mA. At a 5.0 mA level, a
96.7% escape response was exhibited.
An indication of the required voltage drop across the
animal pathway for a given response rate can be obtained
by combining the current response and pathway
resistance data. This transformation assumes that
current sensitivity is not a function of pathway
resistance. A plot of voltage versus response rate for
mouth-all hooves shocks is shown in Fig. 5. The family of
voltage response curves was drawn based on the pathway
resistance percentile distribution. As an example, this
plot indicates that at 1.0 V across the mouth-all hooves
pathway, 90% of the population would respond 28% of
the time, 50% would respond 50% of the time, 10%
would respond 92% of the time.
Fig. 6 gives a similar plot for front-rear hooves shocks.
In this case, there is a base response rate of
approximately 20%. Above the base rate, the effects of
the current can be seen. For example, at 2.0 V, 50% of
the population can be expected to respond 37% of the
time. This represents a 17% response rate above the base
level.
1983—TRANSACTIONS of the ASAE
View publication stats
Reference
1. Azrin, N. H. and Holz. 1959. Punishment and recovery during
fixed-ratio performance. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2:301.
2. Cloud, H. A., R. D. Appleman and R. J. Gustafson. 1981.
Stray voltage problems with dairy cows. North Central Regional
Extension Publication No. 125. Agric. Ext. Service, University of
Minnesota.
3. Craine, L. B., M. H. Ehlers and D. K. Nelson. 1970. Electric
potentials and domestic water supplies. AGRICULTURAL
ENGINEERING 51(4):415.
4. Fairbank, W. and L. B. Craine. 1978. Milking parlor metal
structure-to-earth voltages. Western Regional Agricultural
Engineering Service. H-2.
5. Henke, D. V., R. C. Gorewit, N. R. Scott and D. M. Skyer.
1982. Sensitivity of cows to transient electrical current. ASAE Paper
No. 83-3029, ASAE, St. Joseph, MI 49085.
6. Lefcourt, A. 1982. Behavioral responses of dairy cows
subjected to controlled voltages. J. Dairy Sci. 65:672.
7. Masterton, F. A. and B. A. Campbell. 1969. Techniques of
electric shock motivation. In: Methods of psychobiology. Volume 2.
(R. D. Myers, ed). Academic Press, New York.
8. Phillips, D. S. M. and R. D. J. Parkinson. 1963. The effects of
small voltages on milking plants: their direction and elimination. Dairy
Farming Annual, p. 79.
9. Surbrook, T. C. and N. D. Reese. 1981. Stray voltage on
farms. ASAE Paper NO. 81-3512, ASAE, St. Joseph, MI 49085.
10. Whittlestone, W. G., M. M. Mullord, R. Kilgour, and L. R.
Cate. 1975. Electric shocks during machine milking. New Zealand Vet.
J. 23:105.
11. Williams, G. F. 1981. Stray electric current: economic losses,
symptoms, and how it affects the cows. Proc. 20th Ann. NMC Mtg.,
Louisville, KY. p. 13.
12. Woolford, M. W. 1972. Small voltages on milking plants, proc.
2nd Sem. Farm Machinery and Eng. 41-7.
13. Woolford, M. W. 1973-1974. The effects of intermittent
electric shocks on milk production and animal behavior. Ann. Rep.
Agr. Res. Dir. New Zeal. Dept. Ag. p. 79 (abst.).
14. Woolford, M. W. and P. J. A. Copeland. 1969-70. Effect of
transient voltage pulses on milking performance. Ann. Rep. Agr. Res.
Div. New Zeal. Dept. Ag. p. 13 (abst.).
1511
Descargar